Get me outta here!

Jumat, 17 Juni 2016

Affixes: Prefixes and Suffixes


an affix is a bound morpheme that is joined before, after or within a root or stem.

    Types of affixes
    Prefix: this is added before the root or stem of the word. E.g. unpredictable, unresponsive, revitalize, etc.
    Suffix: a suffix is an affix that is attached to the end of a root or stem. E.g. the plural formatives, -s, -en, -ing, -d, -er, -est, and –less, -ment, -ion, etc.

Affix types

    Infix: An infix is an affix that is inserted within a root or stem. It is added in the base form of a word to create a new word or intensify meaning. E.g.

In Philippines (Tagalog)

buli root, ‘buy’

-um- infix, ‘agentive’

Bumili  ‘ bought’

In Indonesian:

Cerlang  ‘luminous’, cemerlang ‘ brilliant’

    Circumfixes: Consists of two parts- a prefix and a suffix that together create a new word. The prefix and suffix are not considered as separate. E.g. In Dutch berg ‘mountain’à ge-berg-te ‘mountain chain’.
    Simulfix: A simulfix is a change or replacement of vowels or consonants (usually vowels) which changes the meaning of a word: E.g. eatà ate, tooth à teeth etc. 

 In English derivational morphology, suffixes heavily outnumber prefixes. In Chapter 6 we saw that most compounds are headed, with the head on the right. Superficially these two facts are unconnected. Consider, however, the role played by the head house of a compound such as greenhouse. As head, house determines the compound’s 71 syntactic status (as a noun), and also its meaning, inasmuch as a greenhouse is a kind of house for plants. This is very like the role played by the suffix -er in the derived word teacher: it determines that teacher is a noun, unlike its base, the verb teach, and it contributes the meaning ‘someone who Xs’, where the semantic blank X is here filled in by teach. Many (though not all) linguists therefore treat -er as the head of teacher in just the same way as house is the head of greenhouse. This is relevant to the distinction between helpful and *-ful-help. In helpful, the affix is what determines that the whole word is an adjective, and so counts as its head. Accordingly, *-ful-help violates English expectations not just because the affix is on the wrong side, but also because the rightmost element is not the head. In the derived words teacher and helpful, therefore, the two components are not equal contributors, so to speak; rather, the righthand element (as in most compounds) has a special status. Superficially, this view of affixes as heads leads us to expect that prefixed words should be as rare in English as left-headed compounds are (items such as attorney general). Yet prefixes, though fewer than suffixes, include some that are of very common occurrence, such as un- ‘not’ and re- ‘again’. Is our expectation disappointed, then? Not really, despite first appearances. Consider the relationship between helpful and unhelpful. In helpful, -ful has a clearly wordclass-determining role because it changes a noun, help, into an adjective. In unhelpful, however, un- has no such role; rather, it leaves the wordclass of helpful unchanged (see Section 5.6). This characteristic of un- is not restricted to adjectives, moreover. Verbs to which un- is prefixed remain verbs (e.g. untie, unfasten, unclasp), and those few nouns to which un- is prefixed remain nouns (unease, unrest). This strongly suggests that the head of of all these words is not un- but the base to which un- is attached (helpful, tie, ease etc.) – and which is the righthand element. Similar arguments apply to re-: rearrange, repaint and re-educate are verbs, just as arrange, paint and educate are. These prefixed verbs, therefore, are right-headed also. The only prefixes that are unequivocally heads are those that change wordclass, such as de- in delouse (deriving verbs from nouns) and en- in enfeeble and enslave (deriving verbs from nouns and adjectives) (see Section 5.9). So, while left-headed derived words do exist, just as left-headed compounds do, they are also not so numerous as may at first appear.

 More elaborate word forms: multiple affixation Many derived words contain more than one affix. Examples are unhelpfulness and helplessness. Imagine now that the structure of these words ientirely ‘flat’: that is, that they each consist of merely a string of affixes plus a root, no portions of the string being grouped together as a substring or smaller constituent within the word. An unfortunate consequence of that analysis is that it would complicate considerably what needs to be said about the behaviour of the suffixes -ful and -less. In Chapter 5 these were straightforwardly treated as suffixes that attach to nouns to form adjectives. However, if the nouns unhelpfulness and helplessness are flat-structured, we must also allow -ful and -less to appear internally in a string that constitutes a noun – but not just anywhere in such a string, because (for example) the imaginary nouns *sadlessness and *meanlessingness, though they contain -less, are nevertheless not words, and (one feels) could never be words. The flat-structure approach misses a crucial observation. Unhelpfulness contains the suffix -ful only by virtue of the fact that it contains (in some sense) the adjective helpful. Likewise, helplessness contains -less by virtue of the fact that it contains helpless. Once that is recognised, the apparent need to make special provision for -ful and -less when they appear inside complex words, rather than as their rightmost element, disappears. In fact, both these words can be seen as built up from the root help by successive processes of affixation (with N, V and A standing for noun, verb and adjective respectively):

(1)        helpN + -ful                                         →helpfulA

un- + helpful                                        →unhelpfulA

unhelpful + -ness                                 →unhelpfulnessN

(2) helpN + -less                                  →helplessA

helpless + -ness                                   →helplessnessN

Another way of representing this information is in terms of a branching tree diagram, as in (3) and (4), which also represent the fact that the noun help is formed by conversion from the verb:(3) A WORD AND ITS STRUCTURE 73 un help ful ness

N

A

N

V

A


(The term ‘tree diagram’ is odd, because the ‘branches’ point downwards, more like roots than branches! However, this topsy-turvy usage has become well established in linguistic discussions.) The points in a tree diagram from which branches sprout are called nodes. The nodes in (3) and (4) are all labelled, to indicate the wordclass of the string (that is, of the part of the whole word) that is dominated by the node in question. For example, the second-to-top node in (3) is labelled ‘A’ to indicate that the string unhelpful that it dominates is an adjective, while the topmost node is labelled ‘N’ because the whole word is a noun. The information about structure contained in tree diagrams such as (3) and (4) can also be conveyed in a labelled bracketing, where one pair of brackets corresponds to each node in the tree: [[un-[[helpV]N-ful]A]A-ness]N, [[[helpV]N-less]A-ness]N. One thing stands out about all the nodes in (3) and (4): each has no more than two branches sprouting downwards from it. This reflects the fact that, in English, derivational processes operate by adding no more than one affix to a base – unlike languages where material may be added simultaneously at both ends, constituting what is sometimes called a circumfix. English possesses no uncontroversial examples of circumfixes, and branching within word-structure tree diagrams is never more than binary (i.e. with two branches). (The only plausible candidate for a circumfix in English is the en-…-en combination that forms enliven and embolden from live and bold; but en- and -en each appears on its own too, e.g. in enfeeble and redden, so an alternative analysis as a combination of a prefix and a suffix seems preferable.) The single branch connecting N to V above help in (3) and (4) reflects the fact that the noun help is derived from the verb help by conversion, with no affix. At (5) and (6) are two more word tree diagrams, incorporating an adverbial (Adv) node and also illustrating both affixal and non-affixal heads, each italicised element being the head of the constituent dominated by the node immediately above it: help less ness

N

A

N

V

Some complex words contain elements about which one may reasonably argue whether they are complex or not. For example, the word reflection is clearly divisible into a base reflect and a suffix -ion; but does reflect itself consist of one morpheme or two? This kind of uncertainty was discussed in Chapter 2. But, if we put it on one side, then any complex word form consisting of a free root and affixes turns out to be readily analysable in the simple fashion illustrated here, with binary branching and with either the affix or the base as the head. (I say ‘free root’ rather than ‘root’ only because some bound roots are hard to assign to a wordclass: for example, matern- in maternal and maternity.) Another salient point in all of (3)–(6) is that more than one node in a tree diagram may carry the same wordclass label (N, V, A). At first sight, this may not seem particularly remarkable. However, it has considerable implications for the size of the class of all possible words in English. Linguists are fond of pointing out that there is no such thing as the longest sentence of English (or of any language), because any candidate for longest-sentence status can be lengthened by embedding it in a context such as Sharon says that ___ . One cannot so easily demonstrate that there is no such thing as the longest word in English; but it is not necessary to do so in order to demonstrate the versatility and vigour of English word-formation processes. Given that we can find nouns inside

            (5)

Adv

A

V

A

un assert ive ly

(6)

V

re de class ify

V

V

N

nouns, verbs inside verbs, and so on, it is hardly surprising that (as was shown in Chapter 2) the vocabulary of English, or of any individual speaker, is not a closed, finite list.

Minggu, 12 Juni 2016

Nouns derived from members of other word classes



Nouns derived from members of other word classes
Nouns derived from adjectives and from verbs are extremely numerous, and it should be easy for you to think of many other examples on the lines of those given here. Here are some suffixes used to derive nouns from adjectives:
(10) -ity, e.g. purity, equality, ferocity, sensitivity
(11) -ness, e.g. goodness, tallness, fierceness, sensitiveness
(12) -ism, e.g. radicalism, conservatism
All these three suffixes mean basically ‘property of being X’, where X is the base adjective. Of the three, -ness is the most widely applicable, and the great majority of nouns formed with it are not lexical items as defined in Chapter 2. For example, once one has learned DIOECIOUS, one can be confident of both the existence and the meaning of DIOECIOUSNESS. Even so, at least one noun in -ness is lexicalised: HIGHNESS, which
means not ‘property of being high’ (for which we use HEIGHT), but rather ‘royal personage’, as in Her Royal Highness. Some of these nouns are formed from bases other than the free form of the corresponding adjective, e.g. FEROCITY from feroc- (not ferocious), CONSERVATISM from conservat- (not conservative). The FEROCITY pattern is fairly general for adjectives in -ious (compare RAPICITY, CAPACITY alongside rapacious and capacious) but not absolutely general (for example, to delicious and specious there correspond DELICIOUNESS and SPECIOUSNESS, not ‘DELICITY’ or ‘SPECITY’). This gappiness is a reason for counting all nouns in -ity as lexical items, and its implications will be discussed further in Chapter 8. Even more numerous are suffixes for deriving nouns from verbs. Here are just a few:
(13) -ance, -ence, e.g. performance, ignorance, reference, convergence
(14) -ment, e.g. announcement, commitment, development, engagement
(15) -ing, e.g. painting, singing, building, ignoring
(16) -((a)t)ion, e.g. denunciation, commission, organisation, confusion
(17) -al, e.g. refusal, arrival, referral, committal
(18) -er, e.g. painter, singer, organiser, grinder

The suffixes in (13)–(17) all have much the same function (they form abstract nouns meaning ‘activity or result of Xing’), but they are certainly not freely interchangeable: for example, we have PERFORMANCE but no ‘PERFORMMENT’ or PERFORMATION’, and we haveCOMMITMENT, COMMITAL and COMMISSION but no ‘COMMITTANCE’. It is true that some verbs allow a choice of suffixes (e.g.COMMIT), but the nouns thus formed are not synonyms: one can commit a crime, commit an accused person for trial, or commit oneself to a task, but, of the three nouns, only COMMISSION corresponds to the first meaning, only COMMITTAL to the second, and only COMMITMEN to the third. Comparison of ANNOUNCEMENT (corresponding toANNOUNCE) and DENUNCIATION (corresponding toDENOUNCE) confirms that verbs that are similar in shape do not necessarily choose the same noun-forming suffixes (ANNUNCIATIONscarcely exists outside the idiomatic context the Annunciation of the Blessed Virgin). Sometimes a noun’s meaning may even be quite far removed from that of the corresponding verb: for example, IGNORE means ‘deliberately refuse to acknowledge’, yet IGNORANCE means not ‘deliberate refusal to acknowledge’ but rather ‘unawareness’. Of the suffixes in (13)–(17), -ing is the most general, and indeed all verbs can form nouns with it irrespective of whatever other suffixes they may use; but even -ing nouns may have semantic and grammatical idiosyncrasies (one can look at a painting or a building, but one listens to a song rather than to a singing). This semantic waywardness will be discussed further in Chapter 8, along with
a phonological restriction on the use of noun-forming -al. The suffix -er in (18) is the one most generally used for forming nouns denoting a person performing the action of the corresponding verb (agent nouns). But it is not the only agent suffix (TYPIST and INFORMANT use other suffixes), and this is not its only function; for example, DIGGER is more likely to denote a piece of machinery than a person, and we have already encountered -er in Section 5.4 with the meaning ‘inhabitant of ’ (e.g. LONDONER). This is an appropriate place to recall that, although affixation is by far the most common way in which lexemes are derived in English, it is not the only way. Some non-affixal ways of deriving abstract nouns (other than conversion) are:
(19) change in the position of the stress, e.g. nouns PERMIT, TRANSFER
        alongside verbs PERMIT, TRANSFER
(20) change in the final consonant, e.g. nouns BELIEF, PROOF, DEFENCE
        alongside verbs BELIEVE, PROOF, DEFEND
(21) change in a vowel, e.g. nouns SONG, SEAT alongside verbs SING, SIT.

By contrast with some languages, however, the derivational use that English makes of vowel change is minimal. Languages that exploit it much more consistently are members of the Semitic family, such as Arabic and Hebrew.


5.6 Adjectives derived from adjectives
In this category, prefixes predominate. The only suffix of note is -ish, meaning ‘somewhat X’, as in GREENISH, SMALLISH, REMOTISH ‘rather remote’. By contrast, the prefix un- meaning ‘not’ is extremely widespread: for example, UNHAPPY, UNSURE, UNRELIABLE, UNDISCOVERED. Because it is so common, most dictionaries do not attempt to list all un- adjectives. This does not mean, however, that un- can be prefixed to all adjectives quite freely; we do not find, for example, ‘UNGOOD’ with the meaning ‘bad’ (though George Orwell included that word in the Newspeak vocabulary devised for Nineteen Eighty-Four). Another negative prefix is in-, with allomorphs indicated by the variant spellings il-, ir- and im-, as in INTANGIBLE, ILLEGAL, IRRESPONSIBLE and IMPOSSIBLE. It is more restricted than un-, largely for historical reasons such as will be discussed in Chapter 9. For the present, it is worth noting the existence of pairs of more or less synonymous adjectives, one of which is negated with un- and the other with in- or one of its allomorphs:
(22) eatable/uneatable edible/inedible
        readable/unreadable legible/illegible
        lawful/unlawful legal/illegal
        touchable/untouchable tangible/intangible
Such examples confirm that the use of in- is lexically restricted. As the negative counterpart of EDIBLE, UNEDIBLE sounds possible, especially if the speaker has limited education and has not encountered, or has momentarily forgotten, the form INEDIBLE. However, ‘INEATABLE’ as the counterpart of EATABLE is not a form that any English speaker would spontaneously use.

Senin, 06 Juni 2016

Definition of Morphology


Morphology is the study of words and word formation. In this chapter we have considered what a word is and looked at the distinction between word tokens, word types, and lexemes. We have divided word formation into derivation – the formation of new lexemes – and inflection, the different grammatical word forms that make up lexemes.

The short answer to the question with which we begin this text is that morphology is the study of word formation, including the ways new words are coined in the languages of the world, and the way forms of words are varied depending on how they’re used in sentences. As a native speaker of your language you have intuitive knowledge of how to form new words, and every day you recognize and understand new words that you’ve never heard before. Stop and think a minute:

• Suppose that splinch is a verb that means ‘step on broken glass’; whatn is its past tense?

• Speakers of English use the suffixes -ize (crystallize) and -ify (codify) to form verbs  from nouns. If you had to form a verb that means ‘do something the way ex-Prime Minister Tony Blair does it’, which suffix would you use? How about a verb meaning ‘do something the way ex- President Bill Clinton does it’?

• It’s possible to rewash or reheat something. Is it possible to relove,

  reexplode, or rewiggle something?

Chances are that you answered the first question with the past tense splinched (pronounced [splɪnʧt])1, the second with the verbs Blairify and Clintonize, and that you’re pretty sure that relove, reexplode, and rewiggle are weird, if not downright impossible. Your ability to make up these new words, and to make judgments about words that you think could never exist, suggests that you have intuitive knowledge of the principles of word formation in your language, even if you can’t articulate what they are. Native speakers of other languages have similar knowledge of their languages. This book is about that knowledge, and about how we as linguists can find out what it is. Throughout this book, you will be looking into how you form and understand new words, and how speakers of other languages do the same. Many of our examples will come from English – since you’re reading this book, I assume we have that language in common – but we’ll also look beyond English to how words are formed in languages with which you might be familiar, and languages which you might never have encountered before. You’ll learn not only the nuts and bolts of word formation – how things are put together in various languages and what to call those nuts and bolts – but also what this knowledge says about how the human mind is organized. The beauty of studying morphology is that even as a beginning student you can look around you and bring new facts to bear on our study. At this point, you should start keeping track of interesting cases of new words

Rabu, 01 Juni 2016

Lesson Plan Simple Present Tense



LESSON PLAN

School                         : SMP IT BAITUNNUR
Skill                             : Writing
Grade                          : VII/2
Topic                           : Simple Present Tense
Time allocation            : 1 x 40  minutes

A.    STANDARD COMPETENCE
Expressing the meaning in a short sentence that is very simple to interact with the immediate environment.

B.     BASIC COMPETENCE
Revealing the meaning of the speech act is very simple short functional accurately, fluently form of the verb in the sentence relating to the time (at the time). In English there are tenses present tense, but were used tenses Simple Present Tense.

C.     INDICATOR
1.      To differentiate the use of verbs beginning with the subject I, You, We, They, He, She, and It.
2.      To make a sentence with the pattern of simple present tense.
3.      To mention the verb is in the simple present tense.
4.      To identify the meaning of sentente.
5.      To identify the steps of rhetorica the sentence.

D.    LEARNING OBJECTIVE
1.      The students are able to differentiate the use of verbs beginning with the subject I, You, We, They, He, She, and It appropriately.
2.      The students are able to make a sentence with the pattern of simple present tense in accordance to its use.
3.      The students are able to mention a verb in the simple present tense form in accordance with the content.
4.      The students can find out the meaning of sentence.
5.      The students can find out the steps of rhetorica the sentence.

E.     METHOD TECHNIQUE
Grammar Translation Method (GTM)

F.      LEARNING RESOURCE/MEDIA
1.      Paper
2.      Marker
3.      Whiteboard
4.      LCD Projector

G.    LEARNING ACTIVITIES
Learning activity
Activity
Time allocotion
Pre-activity
a.         Greeting and checking the students attendence
b.        Brainstorming on students knowledge about simple present tense
5 minutes
Main-activity
a.         The teacher explains the material specifically
b.        The teacher conducts students to make the simple present tense sentences.
c.         The teacher conducts students to justify the verb is in the simple present tense.
d.        The teacher invites students to mention the simple present tense verbs.
e.         The teacher asks the students to distinguish the verb preceded subject: I, You, We, They, He, She, It.
20 minutes
Post-activity
a.         The teacher and students summarizing what has been learned
b.        The teacher gives a game to the students, the role of games are:
1)      The teacher plays the music and the stduents throw the tool to the next student.
2)      When the music pauses, the students who get the tool have to go forward and make the sentence of simple present tense.
3)      The student have to call two his/her friends to go forward and make the sentences too.
(note: this games were did two times)
15 minutes

H.    MATERIALS
The simple present tense in English is used to describe an action that is regular, true or normal.
The kinds of simple present tense are:
1.      Nominal Sentence
The form:
Positive (+)       : S+to be+O
Negative (-)      : S+to be+not+O
Interogative (?) : To be+S+O+?
The examples:
1.      My mother    is    happy.
         S        To be    O
2.      He is not handsome.
 S   To be       O
3.       Are  they in the classrom?
To be   S             O

2.    Verbal Sentence
  The form:
Positive (+)        : S+V1/V1+es/s+O
Negative (-)       : S+Do not/Does not+V1+O
Interogative (?) : Does/Do+S+V1+O?
The examples:
1.      My father goes to his office by car.
S                V           O
2.      Dina does not drink ice cream.
        S         Aux+V1          O
3.      Do  they visit their friend every Sunday?
Aux   S     V1         O
















                                                                                                Metro, Mei 2016
                                                                                                Teacher Practice


                                                                                                Rizki Dwi Oktaviani